에너지전환 정책 홍보 메시지 프레이밍이 정책 지지도에 미치는 영향 연구 : 프레이밍과 해석수준의 상호 적합성 탐색을 중심으로
초록
본 연구는 탈원전이 핵심인 에너지전환 정책의 홍보 메시지를 어떻게 구성하는 것이 한결 강한 수용자 효과를 불러오는지를 분석한 탐색적 시도이다. 메시지 구성의 요소로서 우선 전망이론(Prospect theory)에 따른 긍정 / 부정 프레이밍을 독립변인으로 설정하였다. 이와 함께 해석수준이론이 제시한 하위 / 상위 해석수준 특성 가운데 어떻게(how) / 왜(why) 요인, 가까운 / 먼 시간적 거리감 요인을 메시지 구성요소에 포함해 이 또한 각각 독립변인으로 설정하였다. 정책이 가져올 이득에 초점을 맞춘 이득위험인식과 정책에 대한 지지도를 종속 변인으로 제시했다. 수용자의 정치적인 보수 / 진보 성향을 조절 변인으로 설정했다. 3개 쌍의 독립변인들이 나타내는 주효과와 이원 및 삼원 상호작용 효과를 살펴보기 위해 연구가설 3개와 연구문제 5개를 제시하고 2 × 2 × 2 요인 실험설계를 하였다.실험결과를 살펴보면 긍정 / 부정 프레이밍, 어떻게(how) / 왜(why) 해석수준, 가까운 / 먼 시간적 거리감의 주효과가 모두 유의미했다. 구체적으로 긍정 프레이밍이 부정 프레이밍보다 더 강한 호의적 반응을 이끌 것이라는 가설은 뒷받침됐다. 전통적 전망이론은 불확실한 잠재적 위험과 관련된 선택에서는 부정 프레이밍이 더 효과적이라고 보았으나 이후 위험의 정도나 유형, 손실 개연성의 정도가 낮아지면 긍정 프레이밍의 영향력이 오히려 더 강해질 수 있다는 방향으로 이론적 발전을 이뤘다. 본 연구에서는 에너지전환 정책의 위험성이 원전 유지 정책보다 작다고 보고 제시한 가설이 뒷받침됐다. 해석수준 특성에서는 어떻게 요인이 왜 요인보다, 가까운 시간적 거리감이 먼 시간적 거리감보다 더 큰 영향력을 발휘했다. 독립변인들 사이의 이원 상호작용으로는 긍정 프레이밍과 어떻게(how) 요인, 긍정 프레이밍과 가까운 시간적 거리감이 각각 조합을 이룰 때 상대적으로 한층 강한 메시지 효과를 나타냈다. 특기할 것은 전통적인 해석수준이론에 근거해 설정한 가설에 어긋나게 해석수준 특성이 다른 어떻게(how) 요인과 먼 시간적 거리감 조합의 상호작용 효과가 가장 컸다는 점이다. 이는 에너지전환 정책의 기본적 속성, 즉 과학기술 영역에 속하고 미래지향적이며 미래 가치가 더 중시되는 특성 때문이라고 해석되며 보다 정교한 후속연구가 필요할 것으로 보인다. 삼원 상호작용에서는 긍정 프레이밍, 어떻게(how) 요인, 먼 시간적 거리감의 조합이 가장 호의적 반응을 불러왔다. 정치적 성향과 관련, 진보적일수록 에너지전환 정책에 대한 지지도가 높을 것이라는 가설은 제한적으로만 뒷받침돼 피상적인 느낌보다는 정치적 성향의 조절 효과가 크지 않았다.
Abstract
This experimental research explores how to formulate PR messages in the ways that will have stronger effect on audience for the energy transition policy, which, in essence, is the phasing out of nuclear power. An experiment was designed to reveal empirically what 'frame' and elements of PR messages for the policy induce the most favorable responses from the participants. Based on the prospect theory, positive/negative framing was set as an independent variable. As suggested by the construal level theory, the message components of how/why and the temporal distance factors of near/far were taken from the properties of the high/low construals and also set as independent variables. In order to measure the effect of messages on audience, benefit perception and policy support for energy transition were set as dependent variables. Participants' political orientation was moderated. A 2x2x2 factorial method was adopted for an multi-dimentional examination of the main effects and binary or ternary interactions among the three pairs of the independent variables. The results of the analyses of 1,112 cases show that the main effects of the three independent variables were found statistically significant. As anticipated, positive framing was confirmed to induce more favorable responses than negative framing. The prospect theory was further developed to acknowledge that, as the degree or type of uncertain potential risk and probability of loss lowers, the effect of positive framing becomes stronger, contrary to early theoretical prediction. As revealed by this result, the prospect theory could be made more practical in policy areas. As for the construal level characteristics from the construal level theory, the “how" factor had stronger influence than the ”why“ factor, while the “near temporal distance” factor had stronger influence than “far temporal distance”. Notably, in the binary interactions, the result showed that the strongest interaction effect was found between “how" and “far temporal distance,” contrary to the hypothesis that was based on the traditional construal level theory. It could be due to the nature of the energy transition policy. Energy policies lie within the realm of science and technology. By nature, science and technology policies tend to be future-oriented and place a higher value on the future. In this study, we attempted to explore how PR messages for the energy transition policy could be made more effective with the help of experimental methods. The empirical and practical implications of this study are the theoretical framework for analyzing message effects. By applying the prospective theory and the construal level theory, the positive/negative framing type and construal level theory based on the prospective theory and how / why dimension. The practical level in establishing an effective message composition strategy was verified because the how / why factor and the type of near / far temporal distance encompassed elements other than where in the specific message component 5W1H (underground principle). It is thought that the connection with the energy transition policy is not great because the message component is related to the spatial location, but it can be applied to the differential analysis of the message effect due to spatial variables such as geographical proximity to the nuclear power plant. From the ternary interactions, positive framing, the "how" factor and the "far" temporal distance were found to have the most favorable responses. The hypothesized moderation effect of political orientation was only marginal as the positive relation between more progressive political orientation and higher support for the policy was found to be limited.
Keywords:
Energy Transition Policy, Positive / Negative Framing, How / Why Construal Level, Temporal Distance, Policy Support키워드:
에너지전환 정책, 긍정 / 부정 프레이밍, 어떻게 / 왜 해석수준, 시간적 거리감, 정책 지지Acknowledgments
This study is based on the doctor’s degree thesis of department of journalism and mass communication, general graduate school, Sungkyunkwan university(이 연구는 제1 저자의 박사학위 논문을 수정·보완한 것입니다).
References
- Boo, S. H. (2013). The effect of temporal perspective and predicted valence on preventive health behavior: The persuasive impact of fit between temporal construal level and message frame. Unpulbished doctoral dissertation, Chung-Ang University, Seoul, Korea.
- Butler, C., Demski, C., Parkhill, K., Pidgeon, N., & Spence, A. (2015). Public values for energy futures: Framing, indeterminacy and policy making. Energy policy, 87, 665-672. [https://doi.org/10.1016/j.enpol.2015.01.035]
- Carnevale, P. J. (2008). Positive affect and decision frame in negotiation. Group Decision and Negotiation, 17(1), 51-63. [https://doi.org/10.1007/s10726-007-9090-x]
- Chaiken, S. (1982, October). The heuristic/systematic processing distinction in persuasion. Paper presented at the Symposium on Automatic Processing, Society for Experimental Social Psychology, Nashville, IN.
- Cho, S. (2012). A study on schema types of energy mix stakeholders : Risk perception for nuclear energy and communication. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, Korea University, Seoul, Korea.
- Choi, Y. (2018). The social media’s effect on political participation: In search of the moderating variables. Journal of Political Communication, 49, 179-214. [https://doi.org/10.35731/kpca.2018..49.006]
- Clark, C. E., Hart, P. S., Schuldt, J. P., Evensen, D. T., Boudet, H. S., Jacquet, J. B., & Stedman, R. C. (2015). Public opinion on energy development: the interplay of issue framing, top-of-mind associations, and political ideology. Energy Policy, 81, 131-140. [https://doi.org/10.1016/j.enpol.2015.02.019]
- Cohen, A. A., Adoni, H., Bantz, C. R., Robinson, D. C., Blumler, J. G., & Gurevitch, M. (1990). Social conflict and television news(Sage library of social research, 183). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage Publications, Inc.
- Converse, P. E. (1964). The nature of belief systems in mass publics. In David E. Apter (Eds.), Ideology and Its Discontents. New York, NY: The Free Press of Glencoe.
- Eom, H. S. (2018). A study on the effects of message frame on the reliability of newspaper articles: Considering the political alignment between the press and the audience. Unpublished master’s thesis, Dongguk University, Seoul, Korea.
- Finucane, M. L., Alhakami, A., Slovic, P., & Johnson, S. M. (2000). The affect heuristic in judgments of risks and benefits. Journal of Behavioral Decision Making, 13(1), 1-17. [https://doi.org/10.1002/(SICI)1099-0771(200001/03)13:1<1::AID-BDM333>3.0.CO;2-S]
- Fujita, K., Henderson, M. D., Eng, J., Trope, Y., & Liberman, N. (2006). Spatial distance and mental construal of social events. Psychological Science, 17(4), 278-282. [https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9280.2006.01698.x]
- Gamson, W. A., & Modigliani, A. (1989). Media discourse and public opinion on nuclear power: A constructionist approach. American Journal of Sociology, 95(1), 1-37. [https://doi.org/10.1086/229213]
- Ha, W. S. (2019). The effects of political ideology on policy support in deliberative polling: Focusing on the mediating effect of the policy frame in the process of deliberative polling for 5th and 6th Shingori. Unpublished master’s thesis, Seoul National University, Seoul, Korea.
- Haider-Markel, D. P., & Joslyn, M. R. (2001). Gun policy, opinion, tragedy, and blame attribution: The conditional influence of issue frames. Journal of Politics, 63(2), 520-543. [https://doi.org/10.1111/0022-3816.00077]
- Han, D., & Kim, H.-I. (2011). Risk and communication: Communication effects on social acceptance of nuclear power. Crisisonomy, 7(2), 1-22.
- Han, J. (2018). Semantic network analysis of news articles on Nos. 5 and 6 Shin-gori public deliberation : Focusing on hankyoreh and chosun-ilbo. ECO, 22(2), 145-175.
- Hwang, W.-D. (2013). A study on the influential factors contributing to social acceptance of nuclear energy. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, Kyung Hee University, Seoul, Korea.
- Kahneman, D., & Tversky, A. (1979). Prospect theory: An analysis of decision under risk. Econometrica: Journal of the Econometric Society, 47(2), 263-291. [https://doi.org/10.2307/1914185]
- Kahneman, D., & Tversky, A. (1984). Choices, values, and frames. American Psychologist, 39(4), 341-350. [https://doi.org/10.1037/0003-066X.39.4.341]
- Kang, B. Y. (2018). A study of the effects of cancer related news frames on cognitive attitude, emotional attitude, and preventive behavioral intention: In application of construal level theory. Korean Journal of Broadcasting and Telecommunication Studies, 32(6), 5-59.
- Kang, Y. G. (2018). The influence of hostile media perception on political and social behavioral intention: Analyses of the mediating role of the third-person perception and the moderating role of political talk. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, Chung-Ang University, Seoul, Korea.
- Kiene, S. M., Barta, W. D., Zelenski, J. M., & Cothran, D. L. (2005). Why are you bringing up condoms now? The effect of message content on framing effects of condom use messages. Health Psychology, 24(3), 321-326. [https://doi.org/10.1037/0278-6133.24.3.321]
- Kim, D. J., Chung, B. H., & Chang, J. H. (2013). A comparative analysis of social and personal level acceptance of nuclear energy: Centered on implications for building public relations strategies. Korean Journal of Journalism & Communication Studies, 57(5), 214-238.
- Kim, H. J. (2017). The roles of prior attitudes and political tendency in selective exposure to nuclear energy information. Journal of Public Relations, 21(4), 56-78. [https://doi.org/10.15814/jpr.2017.21.4.56]
- Kim, H. J., & Kim, Y. W. (2013). The interaction effects of message framing, risk types and audience expertise on risk perception in risk communication: An analysis of radiation risks. Journal of Public Relations, 17(1), 143-183. [https://doi.org/10.15814/jpr.2013.17.1.143]
- Kim, I. S. (2012). The impact of risk perception of nuclear power, perception of knowledge, the use of communication channels, the third-person effect about nuclear accident on optimistic bias Fukushima nuclear accident. Journal of Communication Science, 12(3), 79-106.
- Kim, J. H., Kim, T. H., & Park, I. H. (2010). Effective persuasion message type depending on when people get the results of preventive behavior: Based on construal level theory. The Korean Journal of Consumer and Advertising Psychology, 11(3), 451-474. [https://doi.org/10.21074/kjlcap.2010.11.3.451]
- Kim, K. S., & Yun, S. J. (2010). A exploratory study on acceptability change of nuclear power as a responding solution to climate change as a result of framing effect and its policy implication. Journal of Environmental Policy and Administration, 18(1), 91-129.
- Kim, M. H. (2011). The influence of construal level, message level and temporal distance on consumer's message attitude and purchase intention. Unpublished master’s thesis, Ewha Womans University, Seoul, Korea.
- Kim, S. Y., & Kim, G. S. (2014). After Fukushima: Exploring the attitude change and its determinants about acceptance of nuclear power. The Korean Association for Policy Studies, 23(3), 57-90.
- Kühberger, A. (1998). The influence of framing on risky decisions: A meta-analysis. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 75(1), 23-55. [https://doi.org/10.1006/obhd.1998.2781]
- Lee, B.-K. (2011). The effects of mental simulation and temporal distance on product attitude and behavioral intention in comparative advertising. The Korean Journal of Advertising, 22(2), 129-151.
- Lee, H., & Jeong, I. (2012). Difference in voting intention according to the level of interaction of a politician's twitter. Journal of Political Communication, 27, 261-299. [https://doi.org/10.35731/kpca.2012..27.008]
- Lee, Y. (2010). The effects of voter’s political orientations and political involvement levels on political advertising evaluations: Role of regulatory-focus in processing messages in political advertisements. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, Hankook University of Foreign Studies, Seoul, Korea.
- Leeper, T. J., & Slothuus, R. (2018). How the News Media Persuades: Framing Effects and Beyond. In Bernard Grofman, Elizabeth Suhay, & Alexander Trechsel (Eds.), the Oxford Handbook of Electoral Persuasion. New York, NY: Oxford University Press. [https://doi.org/10.1093/oxfordhb/9780190860806.013.4]
- Liberman, N., & Trope, Y. (1998). The role of feasibility and desirability considerations in near and distant future decisions: A test of temporal construal theory. Journal of personality and social psychology, 75(1), 5-18. [https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.75.1.5]
- Liberman N., & Trope, Y. (2008). The psychology of transcending the here and now. Science, 322(5905), 1201-1205. [https://doi.org/10.1126/science.1161958]
- Liberman, N., & Trope, Y. (2014). Traversing psychological distance. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 18(7), 364-369. [https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2014.03.001]
- Liberman, N., Trope, Y., & Stephan, E. (2007). Psychological Distance. In Arie W. Krugalanski, & E. Tory Higgins (Eds.), Social Psychology: Handbook of Basic Principles (pp. 353-383). New York, NY: The Guilford Press.
- Lim, S. J., Yoon, H. W., Yoon, Y. S., & Sohn, Y. W. (2009). Effective advertisement message based on the expected purchase time and product category: Focusing on construal level theory. Korean Journal of Consumer and Advertising Psychology, 10(2), 321-336. [https://doi.org/10.21074/kjlcap.2009.10.2.321]
- Liviatan, I., Trope, Y., & Liberman, N. (2008). Interpersonal similarity as a social distance dimension: Implications for perception of others’ actions. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 44(5), 1256-1269. [https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jesp.2008.04.007]
- MacGregor, D. G., Slovic, P., & Malmfors, T. (1999). “How exposed is exposed enough?” Lay inferences about chemical exposure. Risk Analysis, 19(4), 649-659. [https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1539-6924.1999.tb00435.x]
- Nisbet, M. C., & Mooney, C. (2007). Framing Science. Science, 316(5821), 56-56. [https://doi.org/10.1126/science.1142030]
- Park, C. H., & Hong, E. Y. (2017). The relationship between risk communication and nuclear risk avoidance behavior: Focusing on mediating effects of trust in government. Korean Public Administration Review, 51(4), 291-322. [https://doi.org/10.18333/KPAR.51.4.291]
- Park, J. H., & Shin, J. H. (2010). Government trust, policy efficacy, and policy support: Residential area CCTV policy in Korea. Korean Institute of Public Affairs, 48(3), 1-24.
- Peoples, C. (2014). New nuclear, new security? Framing security in the policy case for new nuclear power in the United Kingdom. Security Dialogue, 45(2), 156-173. [https://doi.org/10.1177/0967010614521840]
- Perko, T., Van Gorp, B., Turcanu, C., Thijssen, P., & Carle, B. (2013). Communication in nuclear emergency preparedness: a closer look at information reception. Risk Analysis, 33(11), 1987-2001. [https://doi.org/10.1111/risa.12048]
- Prezelj, I., Perko T., Cantone, M. C., Gallego, E., Tomkiv, Y., & Oughton, D. H. (2016). The limits of public communication coordination in a nuclear emergency: lessons from media reporting on the Fukushima case. Journal of Radiological Protection, 36(2), 45-63. [https://doi.org/10.1088/0952-4746/36/2/S45]
- Quick, B. L., & Bates, B. R. (2010). The use of gain-or loss-frame messages and efficacy appeals to dissuade excessive alcohol consumption among college students: A test of psychological reactance theory. Journal of health communication, 15(6), 603-628. [https://doi.org/10.1080/10810730.2010.499593]
- Rabinovich, A., Morton, T., & Postmes, T. (2010). Time perspective and attitude-behaviour consistency in future-oriented behaviours. British Journal of Social Psychology, 49(1), 69-89. [https://doi.org/10.1348/014466608X401875]
- Rivers, S. E., Salovey, P., Pizarro, D. A., Pizarro, J., & Schneider, T. R. (2005). Message framing and pap test utilization among women attending a community health clinic. Journal of Health Psychology, 10(1), 65-77. [https://doi.org/10.1177/1359105305048556]
- Robberson, M. R., & Rogers, R. W. (1988). Beyond fear appeals: Negative and positive persuasive appeals to health and self-esteem. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 18(3), 277-287. [https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1559-1816.1988.tb00017.x]
- Scheufele, D. A., & Iyengar, S. (2016). The state of framing research: A call for new directions. In Kate Kenski & Kathleen Hall Jamieson (Eds.), The Oxford Handbook of Political Communication (pp. 619-632). New York, NY: Oxford University Press.
- Scheufele, D. A., & Lewenstein, B. V. (2005). The public and nanotechnology: How citizens make sense of emerging technologies. Journal of Nanoparticle Research, 7(6), 659-667. [https://doi.org/10.1007/s11051-005-7526-2]
- Shen, L. (2015). Antecedents to psychological reactance: The impact of threat, message frame, and choice. Health Communication, 30(10), 975-985. [https://doi.org/10.1080/10410236.2014.910882]
- Shim, J. S. (2015, August). A study of the effects of conflict frames on the nuclear acceptance. Paper presented at the summer conference of the Korean Association for Policy and Evaluation, Seoul.
- Shin, H., Keum, H., & Chung, S. (2012). The effects of gain and loss frames on individuals’ perceptions of nuclear energy issues : Its interplay with individuals’ value dispositions and materialism/post-materialism. Korean Journal of Journalism & Communication Studies, 56(5), 190-215.
- Slovic, P. (1993). Perceived risk, trust, and democracy. Risk analysis, 13(6), 675-682. [https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1539-6924.1993.tb01329.x]
- Song, I.-H., & Park, S.-Y. (2009). Advertisement effects of message framing and temporal frame. Journal of Institute for Social Sciences, 20(3), 129-150.
- Trope, Y., & Liberman, N. (2000). Temporal construal and time-dependent changes in preference. Journal of personality and social psychology, 79(6), 876-889. [https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.79.6.876]
- Trope, Y., & Liberman, N. (2003). Temporal construal. Psychological review, 110(3), 403-421. [https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-295X.110.3.403]
- Trope, Y., & Liberman, N. (2010). Construal-level theory of psychological distance. Psychological review, 117(2), 440-463. [https://doi.org/10.1037/a0018963]
- Trope, Y., Liberman, N., & Wakslak, C. (2007). Construal levels and psychological distance: Effects on representation, prediction, evaluation, and behavior. Journal of consumer psychology, 17(2), 83-95. [https://doi.org/10.1016/S1057-7408(07)70013-X]
- Tversky, A., & Kahneman, D. (1981). The framing of decisions and the psychology of choice. Science, 211(4481), 453-458. [https://doi.org/10.1126/science.7455683]
- Tversky, A., & Kahneman, D. (1992). Advances in prospect theory: cumulative representation of uncertainty. Journal of Risk and Uncertainty, 5(4), 297-323. [https://doi.org/10.1007/BF00122574]
- Vallacher, R. R., & Wegner, D. M. (1987) What do people think they're doing? Action identification and human behavior. Psychological review, 94(1), 3-15. [https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-295X.94.1.3]
- Wakslak, C. J., Trope, Y., Liberman, N., & Alony, R. (2006). Seeing the forest when entry is unlikely: probability and the mental representation of events. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 135(4), 641–653. [https://doi.org/10.1037/0096-3445.135.4.641]
- Yoon, S. J., & Kim, M. Y. (2018). A study on the construal level and intention of autonomous driving taxi according to message framing. Journal of Intelligence and Information Systems, 24(3), 135-155.
- Yoon, S. W., Ryu, J. S., & Kim, Y. J. (2017, November). Policy decision and its impact on German nuclear phase-out. Paper presented at the fall conference of Korea Technology Innovation Society, Jeju.